Oliver Burkeman 

This column will change your life: Decision Quicksand

Buying toothpaste just got harder, says Oliver Burkeman
  
  

This column will change your life: Decision Quicksand
'Do you want your toothpaste with cooling crystals, whitening gel or a minty stripe?' Illustration: Adam Howling for the Guardian Photograph: Adam Howling for the Guardian

For a while now, it's been dawning on psychologists – and the rest of us – that having more choices doesn't seem to lead to more happiness. The paralysing horror that British people used to experience only as tourists ordering sandwiches in New York delis (wheat or rye? American or Swiss? Ranch or balsamic?) is now a routine aspect of any trip to Tesco. Do you want your toothpaste with cooling crystals, whitening gel or just a minty stripe? Barry Schwartz nailed the basic explanation in his 2004 book The Paradox Of Choice: more choice means more work, and also more opportunities to regret the road (or toothpaste) not taken. Guess what, though? It turns out the problem is worse than we thought.

The hunch that inspired a recent research paper, highlighted on Jonah Lehrer's neuroscience blog, sounds mundane enough. "Our central premise," write the authors, Aner Sela and Jonah Berger, "is that people use subjective experiences of difficulty when making a decision as a cue to how much further time and effort to spend." But that unassuming sentence describes a vicious trap. Our brains, Sela and Berger suggest, habitually use a problem's complexity as a yardstick for judging its importance. Since important decisions tend to be hard, we assume hard decisions must be important, and so we belabour them. Furthermore, this effect, which they measured in a series of experiments in which people were asked to book imaginary flights, is most pronounced when the complexity comes as a surprise – in other words, when people had been expecting a simple decision. Which adds the nasty twist that it's precisely those unimportant-but-surprisingly-complex decisions, the Tesco dilemmas, that we get bogged down by the most. Hence the paper's title: Decision Quicksand.

As Lehrer notes, marketers seem to be getting the message; in the US, the number of new toothpaste brands introduced to the shelves, for example, is actually falling steeply year by year. But the reverse-inference problem – imagining that a decision must be crucial just because it's complex – reaches beyond the supermarket. The field of parenting advice seems especially prone to it. Clearly, there are a million combinations of techniques you could deploy for everything from how to respond to a newborn's crying to what to feed your toddler to the type of books your six-year-old should be reading... but the existence of all those permutations doesn't constitute evidence that it matters which ones you choose. (Childcare gurus with manuals to sell, of course, want you to pour time and money into such decisions, but there is good evidence that parenting style doesn't matter much at all, provided you are, like so many consumers of complex parenting advice, at least moderately well-off.)

Intriguingly, the reverse effect seems to apply, too: another group of researchers found that when a big decision that "ought" to be tough proves more straightforward than expected, people will deliberately find ways to complicate it, just to feel like they put in the effort. The happy flipside of "metacognitive errors" such as these, though, is that just knowing they exist is half the battle. Next time you're faced with some nightmarish choice between different paths, consider the possibility that the right answer might be "any of them". The only wrong answer is to choose none of them and remain where you are, sinking ever deeper into the quicksand.

oliver.burkeman@theguardian.com
twitter.com/oliverburkeman

 

Leave a Comment

Required fields are marked *

*

*